YouTube: https://youtube.com/watch?v=xsGJkSDtLvo
Previous: Why We Love Movie Villains (According to Psychology)
Next: Why Is It So Hard to Remember Things Right Now?

Categories

Statistics

View count:155,281
Likes:8,128
Comments:625
Duration:11:34
Uploaded:2020-09-03
Last sync:2024-03-05 04:00
Quantum mechanics may not seem like it has anything to do with human psychology, but some psychologists are starting to borrow concepts from the field to help make human behavior more predictable.

Hosted by: Hank Green
----------
Support SciShow by becoming a patron on Patreon: https://www.patreon.com/scishow

SciShow has a spinoff podcast! It's called SciShow Tangents. Check it out at https://www.scishowtangents.org
----------
Huge thanks go to the following Patreon supporters for helping us keep SciShow free for everyone forever:

Bd_Tmprd, Jeffrey Mckishen, James Knight, Christoph Schwanke, Jacob, Matt Curls, Sam Buck, Christopher R Boucher, Eric Jensen, Lehel Kovacs, Adam Brainard, Greg, Sam Lutfi, Piya Shedden, Katie Marie Magnone, Scott Satovsky Jr, Charles Southerland, Charles george, Alex Hackman, Chris Peters, Kevin Bealer

----------
Looking for SciShow elsewhere on the internet?
Facebook: http://www.facebook.com/scishow
Twitter: http://www.twitter.com/scishow
Tumblr: http://scishow.tumblr.com
Instagram: http://instagram.com/thescishow
----------
Sources:
http://bear.warrington.ufl.edu/brenner/mar7588/Papers/shafir-reasonbased-cog1993.pdf
http://www.bbc.com/earth/story/20170215-the-strange-link-between-the-human-mind-and-quantum-physics
http://doc.rero.ch/record/315777/files/11238_2010_Article_9202.pdf
https://www.thedocc.com/wp-content/uploads/2015/07/J29.-BuseWang-2015-CD-in-psy_q-cog.pdf
https://www.thedocc.com/wp-content/uploads/2015/02/J17.-WangBuse-2013-QQ-model.pdf
https://arxiv.org/pdf/physics/0302005v1.pdf
https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.1080/14786443208647909
https://www.livescience.com/amp/quantum-like-model-of-decision-making-proposed.html
https://www.pnas.org/content/pnas/111/26/9431.full.pdf

Image Sources:
https://commons.wikimedia.org/wiki/File:Quantum_superposition_of_states_and_decoherence.ogv
https://commons.wikimedia.org/wiki/File:Single_electron_probability_pattern.png
https://commons.wikimedia.org/wiki/File:Wave-particle_duality.gif
https://commons.wikimedia.org/w/index.php?title=File%3AWave-particle_duality.ogv
[♪ INTRO].

We've all seen people act in ways that seem totally irrational —and for most of us, unpredictability is just part of human nature. But for scientists, when something is unpredictable, that's usually a sign that they don't fully understand what's going on.

Which isn't really shocking when it comes to people. Our brains are just vastly more complex than we have the tools to understand right now. But even if we can't understand the brain down to the deepest level, some psychologists think that a set of ideas borrowed from quantum physics could help us make sense of human behavior.

The notion is called quantum cognition—and it isn't suggesting that our brains actually function at the quantum level, just that the mathematical tools of quantum mechanics could help make human behavior more predictable. Now, the idea that this bizarre branch of physics could be useful for understanding the brain might seem like a stretch—but the reason it's useful is because quantum mechanics is all about statistics. For instance, it's impossible to know where an electron is at any given time; you can only know how likely it is that, when you measure it, you will find it in a given place.

And that's not because we're bad at measuring—that's just how the universe works on a fundamental level. Statistics are also useful in other branches of science, though, for different reasons: They can help us understand the big picture even when we don't know all the lower-level details. Like, you can use statistics to predict how a group of people will vote even if you don't know what every individual person will do.

And when it comes to the brain, there are a bunch of low-level details that we don't understand. For example, while neuroscientists have figured out where our short-term and long-term memories are stored, it's still not clear how your brain selects certain details to remember and others to forget. But in some cases, we can use statistics to make decent predictions about how people will behave even if we're not really sure why.

Traditionally, these cognitive models have relied on classical probability, which can be pretty black or white. Like, you either have three aces in your poker hand or you don't. You either blew off that important project or you didn't. …or did you just kind of drag your feet until it was too late?

See, human cognition is full of ambiguities, and classical probability just isn't well-suited to handling them. That's where quantum mechanics might be useful. The quantum world is anything but black and white, but we've developed really powerful tools to deal with that ambiguity.

And recently, psychologists have been exploring whether or not those tools could be reused to help understand the brain. In fact, quantum cognition models are already performing as well or better than classical models at predicting some kinds of human behavior. For example, it's really tricky to predict how humans will make decisions.

Like, no matter how well you know someone, the things they decide just don't always seem logical. In the early 1990s, psychologists showed this with a simple experiment. In it, they asked 98 subjects to guess the result of a coin flip.

If they were right, they'd win 200 dollars, and if they were wrong, they would owe 100. After the first flip, everyone was asked if they wanted to play again. In general, both winners and losers wanted to flip again, which is reasonable since you're more likely to win money after multiple rounds than lose it.

But that was not true for everyone. Specifically, subjects who weren't told whether they'd won or lost the first coin flip mostly decided not to play again. Even though the odds were in their favor.

From the perspective of classical cognition models, this doesn't make any sense. After all, if subjects were told whether they'd won or lost, neither result affected their decision—in general, they all wanted to play again. This is called the sure thing principle because all the options seem to lead to the same result.

But somehow, not knowing made it not a sure thing. And while classical models of cognition struggle to explain how that could be, one principle from quantum physics does offer a way to understand it. See, in the quantum world, just the fact that something is unknown can change the outcome of an event.

That's the premise of the famous double-slit experiment:. In one version of the experiment, physicists fire a beam of electrons at a detector. In front of the detector there's a barrier with two slits in it.

When the beam is turned on, the electrons strike the detector in a pattern that looks a lot like the pattern you get when two sets of ripples interfere with each other. The weird thing is, even if you release the electrons one by one, you still get this interference pattern. In other words, the electron is interfering with itself.

That's because it doesn't exist in a single, precise location, so there's some ambiguity about which slit it passes through. But, if you set up a sensor to measure which gap each electron travels through, this diffraction pattern disappears. So, the instant the electron's position is known, the ambiguity is gone, and the interference is, too.

So, broadly speaking, this shows that in quantum mechanics, simply not knowing can produce a totally unexpected result. Similarly, in the coin-flip experiment, just the existence of doubt changed the likelihood that someone would play another round. As illogical as these scenarios sound, though, neither one is unpredictable.

In physics, scientists use quantum probability theory, which is a model that accounts for the fact that knowledge of something can affect the result. And weirdly enough, you can apply the same theory to human decision-making to predict how people will make decisions, even if we don't understand precisely why. Psychologists were able to use this quantum model to correctly predict people's decisions in the coin-flip experiment, even when the classical model failed.

And they also applied it to the famous prisoner's dilemma. The prisoner's dilemma is a scenario in which you imagine that you and a friend are both arrested for committing a crime. If you both say nothing, you'll each get, say, a year in jail.

If you rat out your friend, you might get off scot-free while your friend gets a maximum sentence. And if you both rat each other out, you'll both get a few years. So, if your friend doesn't say anything, selfishly speaking, it's in your best interest to rat them out.

And if your friend does say something, it is also in your best interest to rat them out. It's another case of the sure thing principle. And yet, many versions of this experiment have shown that if someone doesn't know what the other is doing, they're less likely to snitch.

Even though it's better for them to snitch either way! Just like with the coin flipping, this seems to defy logic— but quantum probability theory can accurately predict that result. In general, though, the way someone answers a question can be unpredictable for a lot of reasons.

In fact, just the order of questions you ask someone has a big effect on the answers they give. For example, imagine that I ask you, “How was your vacation?” And after you answer, I might follow that up with, “How did you get along with your sister?” But if I asked those questions in the opposite order— and if you'd gotten in a fight with your sister, you might say that you liked your vacation less than you would have otherwise. Which, from a purely logical standpoint, is kind of weird.

Like, if you think of your brain as a computer, it already has all the information it needs to answer both questions, and the order of the questions doesn't change that… but somehow the order can still change your answer. And classical models have trouble explaining that, but quantum mechanics might be able to help here, too. See, in quantum mechanics, things that seem like basic math— like multiplication—aren't so straightforward.

For instance, A times B will often give you a different answer than B times A. So quantum models have to account for those weird rules to make accurate predictions. And oddly enough, psychologists have managed to do a similar thing.

By using quantum-inspired math, they built rules that account for order into their models. And because of that, they were able to do something really unusual:. In 2014, they made a specific, numeric prediction about how an experiment would turn out before it even happened.

That's a run-of-the-mill step in lots of kinds of science, but it's really rare in psychology. Psychologists often don't know enough about the underlying causes of a person's behavior to make specific predictions. But in this experiment, a team of researchers analyzed 70 national surveys conducted by Pew and Gallup that randomly ordered pairs of questions.

They made a specific prediction about how the order of the questions would affect the answers, and the results proved they were right! Which suggests there are ways to predict human behavior, even when it doesn't seem to make logical sense. Human behavior isn't the only element of our cognition that can be unpredictable, though.

Even the things we perceive can sometimes seem inexplicable. Think about one of those optical illusions where the same image can take on two different forms. This is called bistable perception, and it's been baffling scientists for hundreds of years.

For instance, this Necker cube was introduced in 1832. Now, you might perceive it as either jutting out of the screen or coming into it—and if you stare long enough, your perception will randomly switch back and forth. But why?

You'd think that one set of input information, like the lines of the cube, should only be able to create one image in the brain. Again, the tools of quantum mechanics can give us a way to understand it. In quantum mechanics, something like an electron exists in multiple positions at once—until you observe it, when those multiple possibilities instantaneously collapse down to one.

This is called superposition, and similarly, when you perceive an optical illusion, it's kind of like you're seeing it in multiple states at once. It's just when you focus on observing the object that those multiple states collapse down to one. And there's more: Different people see different versions of optica l illusions, but each person tends to see one version over the other.

That suggests that the illusion has a certain probability of being viewed in a certain state. And that fits the analogy, because in quantum superposition, an electron has a certain probability of being observed in any given location. So by using this same statistical approach, we can at least characterize the way our brains process optical illusions, even if we don't fundamentally understand why.

For example, in 2007 a team of scientists modeled the brain's response to the Necker cube as a simple, two-state quantum system. That system modeled how often a person's view of the cube flipped from one version to the other. Then the scientists compared the frequency of those flips to two biological timescales: the amount of time it takes the brain to process new sensory input, and the amount of time it takes us to react to that new information.

When they compared this quantum-inspired model to the real results of past experiments, they found that it generally predicted the way people's brains responded to this illusion. Now quantum mechanics and cognition might seem about as far apart as two scientific disciplines can get, but this shows us why it can be useful to look for answers in non-obvious places. Because in all of these situations, our friends in physics can make unpredictable humans a little easier to understand.

Thanks for watching this episode of SciShow Psych! And if you're interested in learning more about the quantum world, you can head over to our main SciShow channel for more videos on that. [♪ OUTRO].