crashcourse
Language: Crash Course Psychology #16
Categories
Statistics
View count: | 2,301,071 |
Likes: | 33,570 |
Comments: | 1,138 |
Duration: | 10:02 |
Uploaded: | 2014-05-26 |
Last sync: | 2024-11-10 01:15 |
Citation
Citation formatting is not guaranteed to be accurate. | |
MLA Full: | "Language: Crash Course Psychology #16." YouTube, uploaded by CrashCourse, 26 May 2014, www.youtube.com/watch?v=s9shPouRWCs. |
MLA Inline: | (CrashCourse, 2014) |
APA Full: | CrashCourse. (2014, May 26). Language: Crash Course Psychology #16 [Video]. YouTube. https://youtube.com/watch?v=s9shPouRWCs |
APA Inline: | (CrashCourse, 2014) |
Chicago Full: |
CrashCourse, "Language: Crash Course Psychology #16.", May 26, 2014, YouTube, 10:02, https://youtube.com/watch?v=s9shPouRWCs. |
You know what's amazing? That we can talk to people, they can make meaning out of it, and then talk back to us. In this episode of Crash Course Psychology, Hank talks to us and tries to make meaning out of how our brains do this thing called Language. Plus, monkeys!
If you enjoyed this video, you'll love our mini-series on Crash Course Linguistics! Watch it here: https://youtube.com/playlist?list=PL8dPuuaLjXtP5mp25nStsuDzk2blncJDW
Want more videos about psychology? Check out our sister channel SciShow Psych at https://www.youtube.com/scishowpsych!
--
Chapters:
Introduction: Kanzi the Bonobo 00:00
What is Language? 1:14
Phonemes, Morphemes, & Grammar 1:48
Receptive & Productive Language 03:15
Babbling 03:55
Early Exposure & Language Acquisition 4:29
Skinner's Operant Conditioning Theory of Language Development 5:51
Chomsky's Theory of Universal Grammar 6:23
Aphasia 7:23
Review & Credits 9:08
--
Crash Course is on Patreon! You can support us directly by signing up at http://www.patreon.com/crashcourse
Want to find Crash Course elsewhere on the internet?
Facebook - http://www.facebook.com/YouTubeCrashCourse
Twitter - http://www.twitter.com/TheCrashCourse
Instagram - https://www.instagram.com/thecrashcourse/
CC Kids: http://www.youtube.com/crashcoursekids
If you enjoyed this video, you'll love our mini-series on Crash Course Linguistics! Watch it here: https://youtube.com/playlist?list=PL8dPuuaLjXtP5mp25nStsuDzk2blncJDW
Want more videos about psychology? Check out our sister channel SciShow Psych at https://www.youtube.com/scishowpsych!
--
Chapters:
Introduction: Kanzi the Bonobo 00:00
What is Language? 1:14
Phonemes, Morphemes, & Grammar 1:48
Receptive & Productive Language 03:15
Babbling 03:55
Early Exposure & Language Acquisition 4:29
Skinner's Operant Conditioning Theory of Language Development 5:51
Chomsky's Theory of Universal Grammar 6:23
Aphasia 7:23
Review & Credits 9:08
--
Crash Course is on Patreon! You can support us directly by signing up at http://www.patreon.com/crashcourse
Want to find Crash Course elsewhere on the internet?
Facebook - http://www.facebook.com/YouTubeCrashCourse
Twitter - http://www.twitter.com/TheCrashCourse
Instagram - https://www.instagram.com/thecrashcourse/
CC Kids: http://www.youtube.com/crashcoursekids
Hank: Baby Kanzi was recently adopted and adjusting to life in his new home. His mother was working with a language coach to learn some English, and Kanzi usually came along, though he didn't appear to pay much attention.
But the language coach noticed that he seemed to be picking up on how to communicate, just by watching his mother's lessons. Oddly enough, it appeared that he was picking things up faster than his mom. For example, the phrases, "you tickle," and "tickle you," meant two different things, and Kanzi's mom...she was having a hard time understanding that syntax.
One day Kanzi was hanging out, playing with stuffed animals, and the coach asked him to make the dog bite the snake. Kanzi put the snake into the dog's mouth, like it was no big deal.
It was a really big deal, because Kanzi is a bonobo. He's actually a language superstar, even among the elite research primates, like Koko the gorilla. Kanzi is the first ape to demonstrate that language can be acquired spontaneously through observation without planned training, and the first to show a rudimentary understanding of grammar, syntax, and semantics.
Again--really big deal. Especially because for years, humans have been proclaiming that it's language that sets us apart from other animals. But are we really alone?
It turns out that question keeps getting more and more complicated. Technically, we define language as a set of spoken, written, or signed words and the way we combine them to communicate meaning. If we change that definition to include complex grammar, then maybe we are alone. But if language is simply the ability to communicate through a meaningful sequence of symbols, as I might do while looking for a bathroom in Sweden or Kanzi does when she's asking for roast marshmallows, well then, welcome to the club, apes.
(Crash Course theme)
We communicate, in part, by engaging our brains and bodies to make sounds that let us transfer thoughts from our brain to other people's brains. But of course, language is more than just making air vibrate with sound-- I can communicate by moving my hands (which you might have noticed I do pretty frequently) or by using visual symbols.
All of these forms of language allow us to comprehend things we've never actually witnessed and exchange information with each other quickly and effectively to, you know, get a job, or be a friend, or use a metaphor. It's hard to imagine a fulfilling life without some kind of language.
Humans have nearly seven thousand different languages, and no matter how different they sound we can break down their basic structure in the same way, using the same three building blocks.
The smallest of them are phonemes. These are very short, distinctive sound units like "ah", "t", "ch", "sh", "f", like, stuff like that. English uses about forty of them.
Phonemes go together to make morphemes, which are the smallest units that carry meaning. This can be words or parts of words, like a prefix or a suffix. For example, the word speech is a morpheme that contains four phonemes: sound units "s", "p", "ee" and "ch".
From there, you arrange morphemes into your language's grammar, or system of rules allowing you to say the things that you want to say.
So those forty English phonemes give us over a hundred thousand morphemes that produce the more than six hundred and sixteen thousand words in the Oxford English Dictionary, which can be arranged into an infinite number of sentences, paragraphs, Wu-Tang lyrics, or Shakespearean plays.
And just as the structure of language starts small, so does how we learn language, and we start very young.
The word infant comes from the Latin "infans" meaning "not speaking". But as early as 4 months they can recognize differences in speech and start to read lips, matching mouth movements with their corresponding sounds, like "ahh," "ee," "i," "oh," "oo." And even at this age you gotta watch what you say about kids in their presence because this also marks the beginning of receptive language, or the ability to understand what's being said both to, and about us.
Soon, that receptive language blooms to accommodate productive language, when instead of just understanding other people, babies start to developing the ability to produce words. Of course that takes a while, but in the mean time they get a lot of practice babbling.
Beginning at about 4 months they start to make all sorts of sounds, although you may get a "dada" or a "mama," babbling is not an imitation of adult speech. In fact, it typically includes sounds from many different languages and a stranger couldn't tell if a kid was Italian, or Kenyan, or Korean by the sound of her babbling. Similarly, deaf babies will watch their parents signing and start babbling with their hands. By about ten months, that babbling morphs into something that begins to make sense, and "mama" probably really means mama.
Without exposure to other languages, a child will actually lose their ability to both hear and create particular tones and sounds that aren't part of his or her household language. So someone who speaks English around the house soon won't be able to differentiate between certain phonemes in Mandarin if they heard them, for instance, or between aspirated and non-aspirated consonants in Hindi.
By the time they're rolling out their first birthday cake, kids will be entering the one word stage of language development. They now know that sounds carry specific meanings, and connect the sound "dog" to that furry thing across the room.
By around 18 months, their capacity for learning new words jumps from about one a week to one a day and by the time they're two, they're probably speaking in two word statements. These choppy sentences are kind of like telegraphic speech. They sound like clumsy texts or old-school telegrams, using mostly nouns and verbs. "Want juice." "No pants." That kind of stuff.
These little sentences make sense, and they follow the rules of their language's syntax. For example, an English speaking child would put an adjective before a noun--"black cat"--while a Spanish speaker would reverse that--"gato negro."
From there the average kid is soon uttering longer phrases and complete sentences, refusing to put pants on, and demanding more crackers! Most humans hit these same milestones during their language development, but there are competing theories about how our infant babbles turn into complex sentences and how we acquire language.
Do you remember B. F. Skinner--the pioneering behaviorist who brought us learning through reinforcement? He believed language was a product of associative principles and operant conditioning. Skinner argued that a kid learned to associate words with meanings largely through reinforcement.
So, in the Skinner model, for example, if Baby Bruno says, "mmmm," and his mother gives him some milk, he will find the outcome--both the milk and the attention--rewarding, and eventually work his way up to saying milk, through these learned associations and shaping processes. It's good to be understood, right?
But as usual, not everyone was on board with Skinner, in particularly legendary American linguist Noam Chomsky. He argued that a kid like Bruno would never reach his full, complex, sonnet-writing potential if his learning was dependent on conditioning alone.
Chomsky instead proposed the idea of innate learning and ubiquitous grammatical categories, pointing out that while the world's thousands of languages may sound wildly diverse, they're actually very similar, sharing some basic elements. He called this "universal grammar."
Chomsky's "universal grammar" posited that all human languages contain nouns, verbs, and adjectives, and humans are born with an innate ability to acquire language, and even a genetic predisposition to learn grammatical rules. Rather than being linguistic blank-slates, Chomsky suggested that we're hard-wired for it from day one.
In the end, though, we're still not sure how we acquire language. However, developmental research into studies of other species have given us a sense that at least some of it is innate, while the role of learning and exposure is also important.
So, if it's true that all humans have some innate capacity for language, where in the brain is it sitting? We've talked a lot about how function is localized in the brain, and that is definitely true for some aspects of language, but while speaking, reading, writing, and even singing all fall under the language umbrella, their locations in the brain are a little more complicated.
Consider aphasia, a neurological impairment of language. People can experience lots of different kinds of aphasia depending on whether they've suffered an injury, or stroke, or a tumor, or dementia. So, maybe they can speak, but not read; maybe they can sing, but barely speak, or write, but not read.
A region of the brain known as Broca's area in the left frontal lobe is involved with the production of speech. If I suffered a trauma to this area, I might still comprehend speech, but struggle to speak. Although, I might still be able to sing, because that's conducted elsewhere in the brain.
On the other hand, if that falling coconut struck another region called the Wernicke's area, a region in the left temporal lobe involved in the expression and comprehension of language, I'd still be able to speak, but my language wouldn't make any sense. So you might find me saying something like "it was two pizza, I called purple brother on the television".
Aphasia and other brain injuries remind us how thinking and language are both separate and intricately entwined. For instance, it's hard to say if non-verbal ideas come to us first and we think of the words to name them, or if instead our thoughts are born in language, or if we'd be able to even think without it. And because language often helps to frame your ideas, your thinking might actually be influenced by which language you're using.
So what are the implications of this if we expand the definition of "language" to include other species? How might Kanzi's ability to communicate that he wants a marshmallow affect his thinking? And how might that thinking influence his language progression and his identity? If only I had the words to describe how fascinating this all is.
If you understood the language I was using today, you learned how languages are built from phonemes, morphemes, and grammar, and when children acquire receptive and productive language and pass through the babbling one-word and two-word phases of development. You also learned some theories on how we acquire language, what brain areas are involved, and how thinking and language are connected.
Thanks for watching, especially to all of our Subbable subscribers. If you'd like to sponsor an episode of Crash Course or even be animated into an upcoming episode, just go to subbable.com/crashcourse.
This episode was written by Kathleen Yale, edited by Blake de Pastino, and our consultant is Dr. Ranjit Bhagwat. Our director and editor is Nicholas Jenkins, the script supervisor is Michael Aranda, who’s also our sound designer, and the graphics team is Thought Café.
But the language coach noticed that he seemed to be picking up on how to communicate, just by watching his mother's lessons. Oddly enough, it appeared that he was picking things up faster than his mom. For example, the phrases, "you tickle," and "tickle you," meant two different things, and Kanzi's mom...she was having a hard time understanding that syntax.
One day Kanzi was hanging out, playing with stuffed animals, and the coach asked him to make the dog bite the snake. Kanzi put the snake into the dog's mouth, like it was no big deal.
It was a really big deal, because Kanzi is a bonobo. He's actually a language superstar, even among the elite research primates, like Koko the gorilla. Kanzi is the first ape to demonstrate that language can be acquired spontaneously through observation without planned training, and the first to show a rudimentary understanding of grammar, syntax, and semantics.
Again--really big deal. Especially because for years, humans have been proclaiming that it's language that sets us apart from other animals. But are we really alone?
It turns out that question keeps getting more and more complicated. Technically, we define language as a set of spoken, written, or signed words and the way we combine them to communicate meaning. If we change that definition to include complex grammar, then maybe we are alone. But if language is simply the ability to communicate through a meaningful sequence of symbols, as I might do while looking for a bathroom in Sweden or Kanzi does when she's asking for roast marshmallows, well then, welcome to the club, apes.
(Crash Course theme)
We communicate, in part, by engaging our brains and bodies to make sounds that let us transfer thoughts from our brain to other people's brains. But of course, language is more than just making air vibrate with sound-- I can communicate by moving my hands (which you might have noticed I do pretty frequently) or by using visual symbols.
All of these forms of language allow us to comprehend things we've never actually witnessed and exchange information with each other quickly and effectively to, you know, get a job, or be a friend, or use a metaphor. It's hard to imagine a fulfilling life without some kind of language.
Humans have nearly seven thousand different languages, and no matter how different they sound we can break down their basic structure in the same way, using the same three building blocks.
The smallest of them are phonemes. These are very short, distinctive sound units like "ah", "t", "ch", "sh", "f", like, stuff like that. English uses about forty of them.
Phonemes go together to make morphemes, which are the smallest units that carry meaning. This can be words or parts of words, like a prefix or a suffix. For example, the word speech is a morpheme that contains four phonemes: sound units "s", "p", "ee" and "ch".
From there, you arrange morphemes into your language's grammar, or system of rules allowing you to say the things that you want to say.
So those forty English phonemes give us over a hundred thousand morphemes that produce the more than six hundred and sixteen thousand words in the Oxford English Dictionary, which can be arranged into an infinite number of sentences, paragraphs, Wu-Tang lyrics, or Shakespearean plays.
And just as the structure of language starts small, so does how we learn language, and we start very young.
The word infant comes from the Latin "infans" meaning "not speaking". But as early as 4 months they can recognize differences in speech and start to read lips, matching mouth movements with their corresponding sounds, like "ahh," "ee," "i," "oh," "oo." And even at this age you gotta watch what you say about kids in their presence because this also marks the beginning of receptive language, or the ability to understand what's being said both to, and about us.
Soon, that receptive language blooms to accommodate productive language, when instead of just understanding other people, babies start to developing the ability to produce words. Of course that takes a while, but in the mean time they get a lot of practice babbling.
Beginning at about 4 months they start to make all sorts of sounds, although you may get a "dada" or a "mama," babbling is not an imitation of adult speech. In fact, it typically includes sounds from many different languages and a stranger couldn't tell if a kid was Italian, or Kenyan, or Korean by the sound of her babbling. Similarly, deaf babies will watch their parents signing and start babbling with their hands. By about ten months, that babbling morphs into something that begins to make sense, and "mama" probably really means mama.
Without exposure to other languages, a child will actually lose their ability to both hear and create particular tones and sounds that aren't part of his or her household language. So someone who speaks English around the house soon won't be able to differentiate between certain phonemes in Mandarin if they heard them, for instance, or between aspirated and non-aspirated consonants in Hindi.
By the time they're rolling out their first birthday cake, kids will be entering the one word stage of language development. They now know that sounds carry specific meanings, and connect the sound "dog" to that furry thing across the room.
By around 18 months, their capacity for learning new words jumps from about one a week to one a day and by the time they're two, they're probably speaking in two word statements. These choppy sentences are kind of like telegraphic speech. They sound like clumsy texts or old-school telegrams, using mostly nouns and verbs. "Want juice." "No pants." That kind of stuff.
These little sentences make sense, and they follow the rules of their language's syntax. For example, an English speaking child would put an adjective before a noun--"black cat"--while a Spanish speaker would reverse that--"gato negro."
From there the average kid is soon uttering longer phrases and complete sentences, refusing to put pants on, and demanding more crackers! Most humans hit these same milestones during their language development, but there are competing theories about how our infant babbles turn into complex sentences and how we acquire language.
Do you remember B. F. Skinner--the pioneering behaviorist who brought us learning through reinforcement? He believed language was a product of associative principles and operant conditioning. Skinner argued that a kid learned to associate words with meanings largely through reinforcement.
So, in the Skinner model, for example, if Baby Bruno says, "mmmm," and his mother gives him some milk, he will find the outcome--both the milk and the attention--rewarding, and eventually work his way up to saying milk, through these learned associations and shaping processes. It's good to be understood, right?
But as usual, not everyone was on board with Skinner, in particularly legendary American linguist Noam Chomsky. He argued that a kid like Bruno would never reach his full, complex, sonnet-writing potential if his learning was dependent on conditioning alone.
Chomsky instead proposed the idea of innate learning and ubiquitous grammatical categories, pointing out that while the world's thousands of languages may sound wildly diverse, they're actually very similar, sharing some basic elements. He called this "universal grammar."
Chomsky's "universal grammar" posited that all human languages contain nouns, verbs, and adjectives, and humans are born with an innate ability to acquire language, and even a genetic predisposition to learn grammatical rules. Rather than being linguistic blank-slates, Chomsky suggested that we're hard-wired for it from day one.
In the end, though, we're still not sure how we acquire language. However, developmental research into studies of other species have given us a sense that at least some of it is innate, while the role of learning and exposure is also important.
So, if it's true that all humans have some innate capacity for language, where in the brain is it sitting? We've talked a lot about how function is localized in the brain, and that is definitely true for some aspects of language, but while speaking, reading, writing, and even singing all fall under the language umbrella, their locations in the brain are a little more complicated.
Consider aphasia, a neurological impairment of language. People can experience lots of different kinds of aphasia depending on whether they've suffered an injury, or stroke, or a tumor, or dementia. So, maybe they can speak, but not read; maybe they can sing, but barely speak, or write, but not read.
A region of the brain known as Broca's area in the left frontal lobe is involved with the production of speech. If I suffered a trauma to this area, I might still comprehend speech, but struggle to speak. Although, I might still be able to sing, because that's conducted elsewhere in the brain.
On the other hand, if that falling coconut struck another region called the Wernicke's area, a region in the left temporal lobe involved in the expression and comprehension of language, I'd still be able to speak, but my language wouldn't make any sense. So you might find me saying something like "it was two pizza, I called purple brother on the television".
Aphasia and other brain injuries remind us how thinking and language are both separate and intricately entwined. For instance, it's hard to say if non-verbal ideas come to us first and we think of the words to name them, or if instead our thoughts are born in language, or if we'd be able to even think without it. And because language often helps to frame your ideas, your thinking might actually be influenced by which language you're using.
So what are the implications of this if we expand the definition of "language" to include other species? How might Kanzi's ability to communicate that he wants a marshmallow affect his thinking? And how might that thinking influence his language progression and his identity? If only I had the words to describe how fascinating this all is.
If you understood the language I was using today, you learned how languages are built from phonemes, morphemes, and grammar, and when children acquire receptive and productive language and pass through the babbling one-word and two-word phases of development. You also learned some theories on how we acquire language, what brain areas are involved, and how thinking and language are connected.
Thanks for watching, especially to all of our Subbable subscribers. If you'd like to sponsor an episode of Crash Course or even be animated into an upcoming episode, just go to subbable.com/crashcourse.
This episode was written by Kathleen Yale, edited by Blake de Pastino, and our consultant is Dr. Ranjit Bhagwat. Our director and editor is Nicholas Jenkins, the script supervisor is Michael Aranda, who’s also our sound designer, and the graphics team is Thought Café.