YouTube: https://youtube.com/watch?v=SZbdK9e9bxs
Previous: Social Stratification in the US: Crash Course Sociology #23
Next: The Personal Computer Revolution: Crash Course Computer Science #25

Categories

Statistics

View count:873,435
Likes:19,921
Comments:460
Duration:10:52
Uploaded:2017-08-22
Last sync:2024-12-01 04:45

Citation

Citation formatting is not guaranteed to be accurate.
MLA Full: "Memory: Crash Course Study Skills #3." YouTube, uploaded by CrashCourse, 22 August 2017, www.youtube.com/watch?v=SZbdK9e9bxs.
MLA Inline: (CrashCourse, 2017)
APA Full: CrashCourse. (2017, August 22). Memory: Crash Course Study Skills #3 [Video]. YouTube. https://youtube.com/watch?v=SZbdK9e9bxs
APA Inline: (CrashCourse, 2017)
Chicago Full: CrashCourse, "Memory: Crash Course Study Skills #3.", August 22, 2017, YouTube, 10:52,
https://youtube.com/watch?v=SZbdK9e9bxs.
This episode is sponsored by The Great Courses Plus. Start your free one-month trial today (and let The Great Courses Plus know we sent you!): http://ow.ly/faUN30db5c7

Crash Course is made with Adobe Creative Cloud. Get a free trial here: https://www.adobe.com/creativecloud/catalog/desktop.html

An essential part of learning and studying is remembering things, so this week Thomas explains some of how your memory works so that you can spend less time working against your memory’s limitations and more time playing to its real strengths.

Resources:
How We Make Memories: Crash Course Psychology #13: https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=bSycdIx-C48
The Magical Mystery Four: How is Working Memory Capacity Limited, and Why?: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2864034/
How to Remember More of What You Learn with Spaced Repetition (includes more detail on the Leitner System): https://collegeinfogeek.com/spaced-repetition-memory-technique/
Herman Ebbinghaus’ original memory study: http://nwkpsych.rutgers.edu/~jose/courses/578_mem_learn/2012/readings/Ebbinghaus_1885.pdf
The Owner’s Manual for the Brain by Pierce J. Howard: https://www.amazon.com/Owners-Manual-Brain-4th-Performance/dp/0062227351
How We Learn by Benedict Carey: https://www.amazon.com/How-We-Learn-Surprising-Happens/dp/0812984293


The Great Courses Plus is currently available to watch through a web browser to almost anyone in the world and optimized for the US market. The Great Courses Plus is currently working to both optimize the product globally and accept credit card payments globally.

***

Crash Course is on Patreon! You can support us directly by signing up at http://www.patreon.com/crashcourse

Thanks to the following Patrons for their generous monthly contributions that help keep Crash Course free for everyone forever:

Mark, Les Aker, Bob Kunz, Mark Austin, William McGraw, Jeffrey Thompson, Ruth Perez, Jason A Saslow, D.A. Noe, Shawn Arnold, Eric Prestemon, Malcolm Callis, Advait Shinde, Rachel Bright, Khaled El Shalakany, Ian Dundore, Tim Curwick, Ken Penttinen, Dominic Dos Santos, Indika Siriwardena, Caleb Weeks, Kathrin Janßen, Nathan Taylor, Andrei Krishkevich, Brian Thomas Gossett, Chris Peters, Kathy & Tim Philip, Mayumi Maeda, Eric Kitchen, SR Foxley, Tom Trval, Cami Wilson, Moritz Schmidt, Jessica Wode, Daniel Baulig, Jirat
--

Want to find Crash Course elsewhere on the internet?
Facebook - http://www.facebook.com/YouTubeCrashCourse
Twitter - http://www.twitter.com/TheCrashCourse
Tumblr - http://thecrashcourse.tumblr.com
Support Crash Course on Patreon: http://patreon.com/crashcourse

CC Kids: http://www.youtube.com/crashcoursekids

  Intro (0:00)


This episode is supported by The Great Courses Plus.

Hi, I'm Thomas Frank, this is Crash Course Study Skills, and if you happen to be watching this at whatever point in the future that we all get neural implants to let us store our memories on servers in space, what I'm about to tell you is woefully inaccurate. Also, do we have flying cars yet?

For those of you who still rely on that mushy gray stuff in your cranium to remember things, though, listen up. Today we're digging into how your memory works and how you can make it work better. At least, I think we are. Nick, we're not filming those makeup tutorials today, are we?

[Theme Music]

  How Memory Works (0:35)


The science of how memory works is complicated, to say the least. After all, how do we explain how a bunch of nerve cells, chemicals, and electrical jolts somehow let you remember algebra, where you left your car keys, and all the lines to The Dark Knight?

Well, it's simple. We, uh...rely on Hank from 3 years ago to do it for us. Seriously, there are two whole episodes of Crash Course Psychology that go through the entire process of how memories are formed and retrieved. But just like Xzibit left to his own devices in a car dealership, I can't resist putting crash courses in your Crash Course. Plus, understanding how your memory works will help you to optimize the way you study. So, let's do a quick review.

Your brain turns information into memories by putting it through a few different stages. The first is sensory memory, which processes pretty much everything your senses detect or experience in the real world. That sensory memory has the attention span of a five year old at the DMV though, so most of what it takes in is lost almost immediately. But what does stick moves into your short-term, or working memory.

This type of memory is sort of like the RAM in your computer; memories don't stick around permanently. In fact, unless you continually rehearse what's floating around in your working memory, it'll pull a disappearing act after about 15 or 30 seconds. This can also happen if you try to cram too much in at once, because your working memory can really only handle about 4 to 7 bits or items of information at one time.

Now you can somewhat increase this limit by grouping bits into chunks, like splitting FBIKGBCIA into FBI KGB CIA, but there's still a limit. Now, all this happens primarily in your brain's prefrontal cortex, but eventually the information has to make its way to other areas of the brain if it's gonna be incoded in long-term memory.

To greatly simplify things, it'll first head to the hippocampus, which augments it with chemicals called neurotransmitters. Along with many other functions, these transmit details about the information-metadata, if you will. Eventually this leads to the formation of new synapses, which are essentially the connections between neurons, though the neurons don't actually touch. Instead they prefer to keep a small gap between each other and let more of those neurotransmitters move information between them.

The whole process of memory formation causes physical changes within your brain: neurotransmitters shuttle all over the place, neural pathways are forged, and neurons themselves undergo structural improvements using proteins such as brain derived neurotrophic factor, or BDNF. And, just like the process of strengthening your muscles through exercise, this all takes time- which is why cramming for a test doesn't work, and why you can't instantly just download jujitsu into your brain like Neo.

As Pierce J. Howard noted in his book The Owner's Manual for the Brain, "work involving higher mental functions, such as analysis and synthesis, needs to be spaced out to allow new neural connections to solidify. New learning drives out old learning when insufficient time intervenes."

Now that you have a bit of an understanding of how your memory works, one crucial tip should be clear: you have to space your learning out over time. But we're not going to just leave it at that, because - as cognitive scientists have known for a long time - the way you do that spacing matters quite a bit. To explain this, let's start with why we forget things in the first place.

Part of the reason is that your brain doesn't encode all memories equally. During the long-term encoding process, the hippocampus will use different levels of neurotransmitters based on, among other things, how important the information is. And this plays a big role in how strongly it's embedded in long-term memory. This filtering mechanism is great for survival, as it allows your brain to safely disregard unimportant things, like what you had for breakfast two weeks ago while paying special attention to what's important, like the fact that there are ninjas behind you right now.

Unfortunately, you can't always consciously decide what's important and what's not, which is why it can be hard to remember all the details from that history chapter you just read. At a primal level, your brain just doesn't think details of Genghis Khan's war with the Quarismian Shah in 1219 are as important as a bear attacking you. However, there are a few tricks you can pull to make it care a bit more.

  Memory Tricks (4:07)


First, understand that your brain latches more readily onto things that are tangible, visual, and uncommon than it does with the abstract or the mundane. Because of this, it can be helpful to develop mnemonics, which are mental devices that help you associate pieces of information in ways that are easier to remember. And mnemonics can take many forms. You can create sayings to remember sequences of letters- such as, "Ernie Ate Dynamite, Good-Bye Ernie." to remember the names of the strings on a guitar. Or you can make up weird stories in your head that includes cues to the information you're trying to associate.

Like, the way I remember that Helsinki is the capital of Finland is by imagining a giant flaming sinkhole in the ground opening up with a bunch of sharks jumping out of it. Since it's weird, it's easy to remember, and it helps me associate the words Hell, Fin, and Sink, which in turn connect with Finland and Helsinki. Additionally, the more connections that lead to a memory, the stronger it'll be - especially if they're learned in different contexts.

When I first learned about caravels, which were those small ships that Portuguese explorers used to travel down the African coast in the 15th century, I had a hard time remembering that name - caravels. But once I started using them in Civ V to build my empire- and to make sure Gandhi never got far enough to nuke me, the memory became a lot more solid, since I was interacting with it in a new context.

Of course, you still have to repeatedly access your new memories once they're encoded if you want them to stick around. This is pretty much the iron law of memorization: except in cases where they're attached to a particularly intense emotional experience, memories fade away unless you repeatedly recall them. Well, sort of. Let's go to the Thought Bubble.

In the 1880's, a German psychologist named Herman Ebbinghaus wanted to understand how memories decayed over time, and he especially wanted to know how long the process took. He began by running countless tests on his own memory, forcing himself to recall long lists of meaningless letters until eventually he came up with the Forgetting Curve.

While largely hypothetical and simplistic in its details, this model demonstrated how memories decay quickly unless accessed again and again. Since Ebbinghaus's days, our understanding of how memory decays has come a long way. And according to the forget-to-learn theory, presented in Benedict Carey's book, How We Learn, memories actually have two different types of strength: storage strength and retrieval strength.

Picture your brain as a library where none of the books ever get stolen or damaged. When a new book is put on a shelf, it's there for good. This represents storage strength, which, according to the theory, doesn't weaken. Once a memory is encoded, the neural pattern can only get stronger. Now, unfortunately, this library has a particularly lazy librarian who doesn't do a very good job of keeping the library's catalog organized. This represents retrieval strength, which does fade with time. Unless you go in and organize the catalog, or recall the memory, you'll eventually lose track of it. Thanks Thought Bubble.

 Spacing Your Learning (6:34)


Now here's where it gets good: the more a memory's retrieval strength has faded, and the greater the difficulty of recalling it, the greater the increase in learning will be. This is called the spacing effect. It's essentially the no-pain, no-gain of the mental world. The harder you have to work to recall something, the greater the reward for doing so. There's an obvious catch though: if you wait too long, the retrieval strength diminishes so much that you won't be able to recall the memory at all. This is where the principle of desirable difficulty comes in.

To maximize the efficiency of your studying, you wanna find the point right before you're about to forget something, and you can do this by using spaced repetition techniques. The general idea behind spaced repetition is to steadily increase the amount of time in between each study session for any piece of information. So instead of reviewing a fact or a concept once every few days, you'd use a schedule like this, where you'd wait one day between the first and second sessions, three days between the second and third, and so on.

To do this precisely, you need a system that tracks your progress in memorizing each piece of information you need to study, since it's never evenly. If you've got a hundred Japanese kanji to learn, it's inevitable that you'll remember some easier than others. If you use the exact same time delays for every kanji, you'll spend too much time studying some, and others won't ever be learned at all.

To solve this problem, you can use the Leitner system. In it you've got five boxes, each of which represents a specific study interval. Box one gets studied every day, box two every three days, box three once a week, and so on. Each fact or term gets its own flashcard, and all cards start off in box one. Once you get a card right, you move it to the next box. And, if you get a card wrong- no matter what box it's in- send it back to box one.

If you play by these rules, you'll ensure that you maximize your efficiency by spending more time studying the cards you have the weakest grasp on. The increasing time intervals of the boxes also helps you leverage the spacing effect, and get close to that point of desirable difficulty.

There are also a ton of space repetition apps for computers and smart-phones that'll let you make the whole process digital. The best known one is probably Anki, which is free on most platforms, but there's also Tinycards, Quizlet, and many, many others. But when it comes to subjects that aren't easily studied on flashcards - like math or even a sport like skateboarding - it's harder to use a rigid, spaced repetition algorithm.

However, the spacing effect applies here just as well, so be sure to space out your practice over time. During any given day's practice, you're eventually gonna hit a wall where you stop making progress; whether its learning derivatives in calculus, or kick-flips in skateboarding, but if you come back to it a few days later, everything will be more likely to click into place.

In each of these study sessions, make sure you're putting the focus on recalling information from your own memory. As we talked about in our video on reading assignments, there are two main kinds of memory- recognition and recall. Recognition is what happens when you're exposed to information you've already seen before and remember it.

But recall involves dredging the information up from the depths of your memory banks without seeing it, which is exactly what you'll have to do in both your exams and in many real-world situations. So when you study, make sure you're focusing on active recall. Don't just passively read over your notes or slides- use them to create quizzes for yourself, or challenge yourself to sit down and write out a summary of what you've learned from memory. 

If you're studying a subject like math or physics, put a huge emphasis on practising with real problems and actually use the concepts and formulas you've learned. In short, studying should feel like work, and it should challenge your brain. When it does, you'll remember more while spending fewer hours at your desk. Thanks for watching, and I'll see you next week.

  Credits (9:46)


This episode is brought to you be The Great Courses Plus, an on-demand subscription service where you can get unlimited access to over 7,000 different video lectures about any topic that interests you, including science, literature, history, math, even cooking or photography. The classes are taught ny award winning professors- from the Ivy League and other top schools around the world.

If you're looking to improve your study skills further, you might like this lecture from Professor Steve Joordens, called Encoding- Our Gateway into Long-Term Memory where you'll learn more about how to improve your own recall. Right now, The Great Courses Plus is offering Crash Course viewers a free one-month trial. Go to thegreatcoursesplus.com/studyskills, or click on the link in the video description below to start your free trial today.

Crash Course Study Skills is filmed in the Dr. Cheryl C. Kinney Crash Course Studio in Missoula, MT, and it's made with the help of all these nice people. If you'd like to keep Crash Course free for everyone, forever, you can support the series at Patreon, a crowdfunding platform that allows you to support the content that you love.

Thanks so much for your support.