YouTube: https://youtube.com/watch?v=EyyDq19Mi3A
Previous: The Spanish Empire, Silver, & Runaway Inflation: Crash Course World History #25
Next: The Seven Years War: Crash Course World History #26

Categories

Statistics

View count:881,411
Likes:10,405
Comments:632
Duration:10:54
Uploaded:2012-07-16
Last sync:2024-12-18 19:30

Citation

Citation formatting is not guaranteed to be accurate.
MLA Full: "Animal Behavior - CrashCourse Biology #25." YouTube, uploaded by CrashCourse, 16 July 2012, www.youtube.com/watch?v=EyyDq19Mi3A.
MLA Inline: (CrashCourse, 2012)
APA Full: CrashCourse. (2012, July 16). Animal Behavior - CrashCourse Biology #25 [Video]. YouTube. https://youtube.com/watch?v=EyyDq19Mi3A
APA Inline: (CrashCourse, 2012)
Chicago Full: CrashCourse, "Animal Behavior - CrashCourse Biology #25.", July 16, 2012, YouTube, 10:54,
https://youtube.com/watch?v=EyyDq19Mi3A.
Hank and his cat Cameo help teach us about animal behavior and how we can discover why animals do the things they do.

References:
http://webspace.ship.edu/cgboer/actionpot.html
http://brainconnection.positscience.com/topics/?main=fa/zebras
http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Myelin_sheath_gap
http://www.bbc.co.uk/science/humanbody/body/factfiles/nervecellsandnerves/nerve_cells_and_nerves.shtml

Crash Course is on Patreon! You can support us directly by signing up at http://www.patreon.com/crashcourse

Want to find Crash Course elsewhere on the internet?
Facebook - http://www.facebook.com/YouTubeCrashCourse
Twitter - http://www.twitter.com/TheCrashCourse
Instagram - https://www.instagram.com/thecrashcourse/

CC Kids: http://www.youtube.com/crashcoursekids

Behavior is action in response to a stimulus. My cat Cameo is now responding to both an external stimulus: the sound of a bag of treats, and an internal stimulus: her hunger, or at least her insatiable desire for treats.

(Intro)


 Introduction (0:23)


Sometimes animal behavior can seem kind of far out, but if you look closely enough, you can see how all behavior serves a purpose: to help an animal mate, eat, avoid predators and raise young.

(0:34) And since behaviors can come with advantages like these, natural selection acts on them just as it acts on physical traits, ensuring the success of animals who engage in beneficial behaviors, while weeding those that do stupid, dangerous or otherwise unhelpful stuff.

(0:46) The most beneficial behaviors are those that make an animal better at doing the only two things in the world that matter: eating and sexing. Still, that doesn't mean that all behavior is just about looking out for number one. It turns out that some advantageous behavior is pretty selfless, actually. More on that in a minute.


 Morphology (1:02)


But first, behavior is really just a product of a pair of factors: Morphology, or the physical structure of an animal, and Physiology, or the function of that morphology. Now an animal's behavior is obviously limited by what its body is capable of doing.

(1:16) For example, Cameo does not have opposable thumbs, so as much as she would like to get into the treat bag by herself, she cannot. This limitation is strictly hereditary: no cats can open treat bags with their thumbs, cause no cats have opposable thumbs. So, much as she would like to get into the treat bag by herself, she cannot. This limitation is strictly hereditary, no cats can open treat bags with their thumbs, because no cats have opposable thumbs. Though some cats do have thumbs! In the same way a penguin can't fly to escape a predator, or a gazelle can't, you know, reach the same leaves as a giraffe can. 


 Physiology (1:38)


Similarly, behavior is constrained by an animal's physiology, like Cameo is built for chasing down little critters and eating meat, not beds of lettuce. This is because her physiology, everything from her teeth to her digestive system, are geared for eating meat, and if she like, pounced on and ate every blade of grass she came across, let's just say that I would not want to be in charge of that litter box. 

(2:01) Now the traits that make up an animal's morphology and physiology are often heritable, so we generally talk about selection acting on those traits. But as natural selection hones these traits, it's really selecting their associated behaviors. It's the use of the trait, using wings and feathers to escape predators or using a long neck to reach leaves, that provides the evolutionary advantage. 

(2:20) Still, that doesn't mean that all behavior is coded in an animals genes, some behaviors are learned. And even for animals that learn how to do things, natural selection has favored brain structures that are capable of learning. So one way or another, most behaviors have some genetic underpinning, and we call those behaviors adaptive. 

(2:36) Problem is, it's not always obvious what the evolutionary advantages are for some of the nutty things that animals do. Like, why does a snapping turtle always stick out its tongue? How does a tiny Siberian hamster find its mate, miles across the unforgiving tundra? Why does a bower bird collect piles of garbage? 

(2:54) To answer questions like those, we have to figure out 1) What stimulus causes these behaviors, and 2) what functions the behaviors serve. To do this, I'm going to need the help of one of the first animal behavior scientists ever, or ethologists, Niko Tinbergen. Tinbergen developed a set of four questions aimed at understanding animal behavior. The questions focus on how a behavior occurs, and why natural selection has favored this particular behavior.


 Proximate Causes (3:16)


Determining how a behavior occurs actually involves two questions, 1) What stimulus causes it? and 2) What does the animal's body do in response to that stimulus? These are the causes that are closest to the specific behavior that we're looking at, so they're called the proximate causes.

(3:33) In the case of the male Siberian hamster, the stimulus is a delicious smelling pheromone that the sexy female hamster releases when she's ready to mate. The male hamster's response, of course, is to scuttle, surprisingly, over several miles if necessary to find and mate with her. So the proximate cause of this behavior was that the girl hamster signalled that she was ready to knock boots, and the male ran like crazy to get to the boot-knockin'. 


 Ultimate Causes (3:56)


Asking the more complex question of why natural selection has favored this behavior requires asking two more questions: 1) what about this behavior helps this animal survive and/or reproduce? and 2) What is the evolutionary history of this behavior? These, as you can tell, are bigger picture questions, and they show us the ultimate causes of the behavior. 

(4:17) The answer to the first question, of course, is that the ability of a male hamster to detect and respond to the pheromones of an ovulating female is directly linked to his reproductive success. As for the second question, you can also see that male hamsters with superior pheromone detectors will be able to find females more successfully than other male hamsters, and thereby produce more offspring. So natural selection has honed this particular physical ability and behavior over generations of hamsters.

(4:41) So, who would have thought to ask these questions in the first place? And where's my chair? 

 Biolo-graphy: Niko Tinbergen (4:50)


Niko Tinbergen was one third of a trifecta of revolutionary ethologists in the 20th century. Along with Austrians Karl von Frisch and Konrad Lorenz, he provided a foundation for studying animal behavior and applied these ideas to the study of specific behaviors and for that all three shared the Nobel Prize in 1973. 

(5:10) You may have seen the famous photos of young greylag geese following obediently in a line behind a man. That was Lorenz, and his experiments first conducted in the 1930s introduced the world to imprinting, the formation of social bonds in infant animals, and the behavior that includes both learned and innate components. 

(5:28) When he observed newly hatched ducklings and geese, he discovered that waterfowl in particular had no innate recognition of their mothers. In the case of greylag geese, he found the imprinting stimulus to be any nearby object moving away from the young. So, when incubator-hatched goslings spent their first hours with Lorenz, not only did they follow him, but they showed no recognition of their real mother or other adults in their species. 

(5:51) Unfortunately, Lorenz was also a member of the Nazi party from 1938 to 1943. And in response to some of his studies on degenerative features that arose in hybrid geese, Lorenz warned that it took only a small amount of "tainted blood" to have an influence on a "pure-blooded" race. 

(6:07) Unsurprisingly, Nazi party leaders were quick to draw some insane conclusions from Lorenz's behavioral studies in the cause of what they called race hygiene. Lorenz never denied his Nazi affiliation but spent years trying to distance himself from the party and apologizing for getting caught up in that evil. 


 Foraging Behavior (6:24)


Now how exactly does natural selection act on behavior out there in the world? That's where we turn to those two types of behavior that are the only things in the world that matter: eating and sex-having.

(6:34) Behavior associated with finding and eating food is known as foraging, which you've heard of, and natural selection can act on behaviors that allow animals to exploit food sources while using the least amount of energy possible. This sweet spot is known as the optimal foraging model. And the alligator snapping turtle has optimal foraging all figured out; rather than running around hunting down its prey, it simply sits in the water, and food comes to him. 

(6:57) See, the alligator snapping turtle has a long, pink tongue divided into two segments, making it look like a tasty worm to a passing fish. In response to the stimulus of a passing fish, it sticks out it's tongue and wiggles it. Natural selection has, over many generations acted not only on turtles with pinker and more wiggly tongues to catch more fish, it's also acted on those that best know how and when to wiggle those tongues to get the most food. So it's selecting both the physical trait and the behavior that best exploits it. 


 Sexual Selection (7:27)


And what could be sexier than a turtle's wiggly tongue dance? Well, how about sex? As we saw with our friend the horny Siberian hamster, some behaviors and their associated physical features are adapted to allow an animal to reproduce more, simply by being better at finding mates. But many times, animals of the same species live close together or in groups, and determining who in what group gets to mate creates some interesting behaviors and features. 

(7:50) This is what sexual selection is all about. Often, males of a species will find and defend a desirable habitat to raise young in, and females will choose a male based on their territory. But what about those species, and there are many of them, where the female picks a male not because of that, but because of how he dances, or even weirder, how much junk he's collected? 

(8:10) Take the male bower bird. He builds an elaborate hut, or bower, out of twigs and bits of grass, then spends an enormous amount of time collecting stuff, sometimes piles of berries, and sometimes piles of pretty, blue, plastic crap. Ethologists believe that he's collecting the stuff to attract the female to check out his elaborate house. 

(8:31) Once the female's been enticed to take a closer look, the male starts to sing songs and dance around, often mimicking other species, inside of his little house for her. Females will inspect a number of these bowers before choosing who to mate with. 

(8:45) Now doing more complex dances and having more blue objects in your bower scores bigger with females. And ethologists have shown that a higher level of problem solving, or intelligence, in males correlates to both of these activities. So yeah, it took some time to brawn to build that bower and collect all that junk, but chicks also dig nerds who can learn dances. So the bowerbird's brain is evolving in response to sexual selection by females. This intelligence likely also translates into other helpful behaviors like avoiding predators.


 Altruism (9:11)


So thanks to the evolution of behavior, we're really good at taking care of our nutritional and sexual needs. But what's confused scientists for a long time is why animals often look after others' needs. 

(9:21) For instance, vampire bats in South America will literally regurgitate blood into the mouths of members of its clan who didn't get a meal that night. How do you explain animals who act altruistically like that? We actually did a whole SciShow episode on this very subject  but basically, we can thank British scientist William Hamilton for coming up with an equation to explain how natural selection can simultaneously make animals fit and allow for the evolution of altruism. 

(9:46) Hamilton found that the evolution of altruism was best understood at the level of larger communities, especially extended animal families. Basically, altruism can evolve if the benefit of a behavior is greater than its cost on an individual, because it helped the individual's relatives enough to make it worth it. Hamilton called this inclusive fitness, expanding Darwin's definition of fitness basically, how many babies somebody's making to include the offspring of relatives. 

(10:10) (to Cameo and Lemon) So I guess the only question left is, if I forget to feed you two, who is going to regurgitate blood into the other one's mouth? Yeah, there's probably a reason why that only happens with bats.


 Conclusion (10:20)


Thank you for watching this episode of CrashCourse biology. Thank you to Cameo for being such a good kitty. Yeah, she finally gets her treats. There's a table of contents, of course. If you want to reinforce any of the knowledge that you gained today. If you have questions or ideas for us you can get in touch with us on Facebook or Twitter, or of course, in the comments below.  We'll see you next time.